How the human ventral striatum kicks in during decision-making
Emory Health Sciences
The brain's striatum (shown in red) is a critical component of movement and reward systems. (Wikipedia Commons) |
Nature Human Behavior published the research by scientists at Emory University.
It gives the first detailed
view of ventral striatum activity during three phases of effort-based
decision-making -- the anticipation of initiating an effort, the actual
execution of the effort and the reward, or outcome, of the effort.
"It's important to understand the neural mechanisms underlying motivation," says Shosuke Suzuki, first author of the study and an Emory graduate student of psychology.
"Our work has wide implications for treatment of disorders
related to reduced motivation, such as depression, schizophrenia and PTSD. It
may also help enhance motivational programs for everything from education to athletics
and public health."
"The willingness to expend effort is something crucial to our survival and something that we use every day," adds Michael Treadway, senior author of the study and Winship Distinguished Research Professor in Emory's Department of Psychology and Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science.
"We've
identified two closely overlapping, but nevertheless distinct, areas of the
ventral striatum involved with different phases of effort-based
decision-making. And we've provided a concrete neuroimaging tool to measure the
sensitivity of signals associated with these phases that others can apply to
their own data."
For
example, Treadways says, the new method could provide a window into how a drug
is affecting the brains of patients suffering from motivational deficits,
compared to controls.
Treadway's
lab focuses on understanding the molecular and circuit-level mechanisms of
psychiatric symptoms related to mood disorders, anxiety and decision-making.
The
ventral striatum, located deep within the brain's cerebral hemispheres, is an
area associated with movement and mediating rewarding experiences and
motivation.
Neuroimaging
has consistently shown that the ventral striatum activates during
decision-making to encode the potential value of rewards relative to costs,
such as wait times and probability. The ventral striatum helps you decide
whether to pay more for "next-day" delivery or choose "free,
one-week" delivery to receive a package.
Neuroimaging
studies had previously failed, however, to detect a strong value signal in the
ventral striatum for decisions that require a physical effort. If you want more
coffee, but the pot is empty, is it worth getting up and brewing some more?
"It
was a mystery why this brain region encoded the value of a reward versus time
and probability but did not appear to do so for physical effort," Suzuki
says. "It's been a paradox in the neuroimaging literature."
Previous research on rodents showed that the ventral striatum is critical for motivating an animal to work for rewards like food. Animal research also shows evidence for two opposing signals in the ventral striatum.
An activation signal prepares
an animal to work and a discounting signal helps an animal select rewards that
require the least effort. These signals help animals work for what they need,
while also making sure they don't work more than they have to.
The presence of these signals had never been tested in humans. The Emory researchers theorized that as the physical cost to perform a task rises, the activation signal would drive an increase in activity in the ventral striatum, while the discounting signal would drive a decrease.
They proposed that the
simultaneous firing of these two signals -- the cost of effort versus the
activation of effort itself -- is what made it harder to detect the value
signal in previous studies.
An
additional complication to detecting brain activation associated with physical
effort is the fact that neuroimaging requires participants to lie still within
a functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) machine while their brains are
scanned.
To
get around these issues, the researchers designed fMRI experiments that would
allow participants to remain in a supine position and would also separate the
neural signals involving effort from the one associated with the cost of the
effort.
For the first set of experiments, the researchers created a virtual maze. As their brains were scanned, study participants were presented with maze navigation tasks that required different levels of effort. In one condition, the participants watched themselves move through the virtual maze passively.
In another
condition, they simply pressed a button on a handheld device to move through
the maze. A third condition required the higher effort of repeatedly and
rapidly pressing the button to move through the maze. Each maze, when
successfully completed, rewarded them with a nominal dollar amount.
During
a second experiment, the neural activity of participants was measured as they
made a series of choices between two options, with varying amounts of reward
and effort required for each option. The effort and reward amounts were
presented sequentially to try to isolate the effort-activation signal during
the anticipation of various effort demands.
The results showed that two distinct regions of the ventral striatum fired in response to different phases of physical effort and effort-based decision making, with some overlap.
Activity in an anterior region was mainly associated
with reward and effort costs, while activity in a dorsal region was mainly
associated with initiation of effortful movement. And this activity related to
effortful movement was distinct from activity in another region, called the
putamen, which was associated with initiation of simple movement.
The
researchers now hope to build upon this increased awareness for how the brain
encodes signals related to motivation.
"Our current paper provides a paradigm for how to measure brain activity for effort-based decisions associated with assigned tasks," Suzuki says.
"Now we're developing experiments to identify specific modes of signaling
when people spontaneously initiate action. That may give us a better measure of
how the brain operates when people do things because they want to do them, in
real-life situations. Getting sensitive measurements for how people normally
decide to expend effort may help us develop better treatments for people
suffering motivational deficits related to depression or other illnesses."