Baycrest Centre for Geriatric Care
A research team from the Rotman Research Institute at Baycrest
Health Sciences has shown for the first time that these different ways of
experiencing the past are associated with distinct brain connectivity patterns
that may be inherent to the individual and suggest a life-long 'memory trait'.
The study was recently published online in the journal Cortex.
"For decades, nearly all research on memory and brain
function has treated people as the same, averaging across individuals,"
said lead investigator Dr. Signy Sheldon, now an assistant professor of
Psychology at McGill University.
In the study, 66 healthy young adults (average age 24) completed
an online questionnaire -- the Survey of Autobiographical Memory (SAM) --
describing how well they remember autobiographical events and facts.
Their
responses fell between the extremes seen in people with Highly Superior
Autobiographical Memory (HSAM) or Severely Deficient Autobiographical Memory
(SDAM) recently described by memory researchers. This allowed researchers to
study normal variation in autobiographical memory.
After filling out the online survey, the 66 participants had
their brains scanned at Baycrest with resting state functional magnetic resonance
imaging, a technique that maps patterns of brain connectivity, or how activity
correlates across different brain regions.
The researchers focused on connections between the brain's
medial temporal lobes and other brain regions. The medial temporal lobes are
well known to be fundamentally involved with memory function.
Those who
endorsed richly-detailed autobiographical memories had higher medial temporal
lobe connectivity to regions at the back of the brain involved in visual
processes, whereas those tending to recall the past in a factual manner (minus
the rich details) showed higher medial temporal lobe connectivity to areas at
the front of the brain involved in organization and reasoning.
The findings raise interesting questions for cognitive scientists,
related to aging and brain health. One of the more provocative inquiries: could
certain memory traits be protective, delaying the manifestation of age-related
cognitive decline in later years?
"With aging and early dementia, one of the first things that
people notice is difficulty retrieving the details of events," said the
study's senior author Dr. Brian Levine, a senior scientist at Baycrest's Rotman
Research Institute and professor of Psychology at the University of Toronto.
"Yet no one has looked at how this relates to memory
traits. People who are used to retrieving richly-detailed memories may be very
sensitive to subtle memory changes as they age, whereas those who rely on a
factual approach may prove to be more resistant to such changes," he said.
Could a person's profile of memory traits help guide treatment
of memory issues in later life? The Rotman findings open the door to exciting
possibilities that require further scientific exploration, said Dr. Levine.
Follow-up studies are now being conducted relating memory traits to
personality, psychiatric conditions such as depression, performance on other
cognitive measures, and genetics.
This research is part of a new trend in focusing on differences
in brain structure and function across healthy people. It is the first to
relate such brain differences to differences in everyday autobiographical
memory functioning.