Music
really is a universal language
Cell Press
To watch this amazing performance on YouTube:
This is one of my favorite examples of the universality of music. Pavarotti's final public performance before his death at the 2006 Olympic games.
Every culture enjoys music and song, and those songs serve many different purposes: accompanying a dance, soothing an infant, or expressing love. Now, after analyzing recordings from all around the world, researchers reporting in Current Biology on January 25 show that vocal songs sharing one of those many functions tend to sound similar to one another, no matter which culture they come from.
As a result, people listening to those songs in any one of 60 countries could make accurate inferences about them, even after hearing only a quick 14-second sampling.
The findings are
consistent with the existence of universal links between form and function in
vocal music, the researchers say.
"Despite the
staggering diversity of music influenced by countless cultures and readily
available to the modern listener, our shared human nature may underlie basic
musical structures that transcend cultural differences," says Samuel Mehr
(@samuelmehr) at Harvard University.
"We show that our
shared psychology produces fundamental patterns in song that transcend our
profound cultural differences," adds co-first author of the study Manvir
Singh, also at Harvard. "This suggests that our emotional and behavioral responses
to aesthetic stimuli are remarkably similar across widely diverging
populations."
Across the animal kingdom, there are links between form and function in vocalization. For instance, when a lion roars or an eagle screeches, it sounds hostile to naive human listeners. But it wasn't clear whether the same concept held in human song.
Many people believe
that music is mostly shaped by culture, leading them to question the relation
between form and function in music, Singh says. "We wanted to find out if
that was the case or not."
In their first
experiment, Mehr and Singh's team asked 750 internet users in 60 countries to
listen to brief, 14-second excerpts of songs. The songs were selected
pseudo-randomly from 86 predominantly small-scale societies, including hunter-gatherers,
pastoralists, and subsistence farmers. Those songs also spanned a wide array of
geographic areas designed to reflect a broad sampling of human cultures.
After listening to
each excerpt, participants answered six questions indicating their perceptions
of the function of each song on a six-point scale.
Those questions evaluated the degree to which listeners believed that each song was used (1) for dancing, (2) to soothe a baby, (3) to heal illness, (4) to express love for another person, (5) to mourn the dead, and (6) to tell a story. (In fact, none of the songs were used in mourning or to tell a story. Those answers were included to discourage listeners from an assumption that only four song types were actually present.)
Those questions evaluated the degree to which listeners believed that each song was used (1) for dancing, (2) to soothe a baby, (3) to heal illness, (4) to express love for another person, (5) to mourn the dead, and (6) to tell a story. (In fact, none of the songs were used in mourning or to tell a story. Those answers were included to discourage listeners from an assumption that only four song types were actually present.)
In total, participants
listened to more than 26,000 excerpts and provided more than 150,000 ratings
(six per song). The data show that, despite participants' unfamiliarity with
the societies represented, the random sampling of each excerpt, their very
short duration, and the enormous diversity of this music, the ratings
demonstrated accurate and cross-culturally reliable inferences about song
functions on the basis of song forms alone.
In a second, follow-up
experiment designed to explore possible ways in which people made those determinations
about song function, the researchers asked 1,000 internet users in the United
States and India to rate the excerpts for three "contextual"
features: (1) number of singers, (2) gender of singer(s), and (3) number of
instruments. They also rated them for seven subjective musical features: (1)
melodic complexity, (2) rhythmic complexity, (3) tempo, (4) steady beat, (5)
arousal, (6) valence, and (7) pleasantness.
An analysis of those
data showed that there was some relationship between those various features and
song function. But it wasn't enough to explain the way people were able to so
reliably detect a song's function.
Mehr and Singh say
that one of the most intriguing findings relates to the relationship between
lullabies and dance songs. "Not only were users best at identifying songs
used for those functions, but their musical features seem to oppose each other
in many ways," Mehr says.
Dance songs were generally faster, rhythmically and melodically complex, and perceived by participants as "happier" and "more exciting"; lullabies, on the other hand, were slower, rhythmically and melodically simple, and perceived as "sadder" and "less exciting."
Dance songs were generally faster, rhythmically and melodically complex, and perceived by participants as "happier" and "more exciting"; lullabies, on the other hand, were slower, rhythmically and melodically simple, and perceived as "sadder" and "less exciting."
The researchers say
they are now conducting these tests in listeners who live in isolated,
small-scale societies and have never heard music other than that of their own
cultures. They are also further analyzing the music of many cultures to try to
figure out how their particular features relate to function and whether those
features themselves might be universal.