New methods
help identify what drives sensitive or socially unacceptable behaviors
Colorado State University
Conservation scientists and
statisticians at Colorado State University have teamed up to solve a key
problem for the study of sensitive behaviors like poaching, harassment,
bribery, and drug use.
Sensitive behaviors -- defined as
socially unacceptable or not compliant with rules and regulations -- are
notoriously hard to study, researchers say, because people often do not want to
answer direct questions about them.
To overcome this challenge,
scientists have developed indirect questioning approaches that protect
responders' identities.
However, these methods also make it difficult to predict which sectors of a population are more likely to participate in sensitive behaviors, and which factors, such as knowledge of laws, education, or income, influence the probability that an individual will engage in a sensitive behavior.
However, these methods also make it difficult to predict which sectors of a population are more likely to participate in sensitive behaviors, and which factors, such as knowledge of laws, education, or income, influence the probability that an individual will engage in a sensitive behavior.
Assistant Professor Jennifer Solomon and Associate Professor Michael Gavin of the Department of Human Dimensions of Natural Resources at CSU, and Abu Conteh from MacEwan University in Alberta, Canada, have teamed up with Professor Jay Breidt and doctoral student Meng Cao in the CSU Department of Statistics to develop a new method to solve the problem.
The study, "Understanding the
drivers of sensitive behavior using Poisson regression from quantitative
randomized response technique data," was published recently in PLOS
One.
Conteh, who, as a doctoral student,
worked with Gavin in New Zealand, used a specific technique, known as
quantitative randomized response, to elicit confidential answers to questions
on behaviors related to non-compliance with natural resource regulations from a
protected area in Sierra Leone.
In this technique, the researcher
conducting interviews has a large container containing pingpong balls, some
with numbers and some without numbers. The interviewer asks the respondent to
pick a ball at random, without revealing it to the interviewer.
If the ball has a number, the respondent tells the interviewer the number. If the ball does not have a number, the respondent reveals how many times he illegally hunted animals in a given time period.
If the ball has a number, the respondent tells the interviewer the number. If the ball does not have a number, the respondent reveals how many times he illegally hunted animals in a given time period.
While the interviewer does not know
specifically if the number given was a number on a ball or a number related to
illegal hunting, the distribution of numbered and unnumbered balls allows
inferences to be drawn.
Using this technique, Conteh made
inferences about the percent of the population that engaged in illegal
behavior. But Gavin, Solomon and Conteh wanted to learn more about what drives
poaching:
Are younger men more inclined to engage in this illegal activity? Are people from larger households more likely to violate hunting regulations? Were people deterred from illegal activity by their knowledge of law enforcement activity?
Are younger men more inclined to engage in this illegal activity? Are people from larger households more likely to violate hunting regulations? Were people deterred from illegal activity by their knowledge of law enforcement activity?
Frustrated with their inability to
move beyond generalizations about an overall population, Gavin and Solomon
asked Breidt for help refining the data.
An intriguing proposition
Breidt had read about the randomized
response technique in scientific journals, and was intrigued.
"Here's this interesting
probabilistic way to get data, but I had never encountered someone who really
did it," he said.
He enlisted Cao, who is pursuing a doctoral
degree in statistics, to tackle the problem. Cao and Breidt developed a
methodology and the software to implement it. They tested the new methodology
with extensive simulations, before applying it to the Sierra Leone data.
Armed with the new computer program,
the scientists found that people from rural communities with less access to
jobs in urban centers were more likely to hunt in the reserve. People in
communities with a greater proportion people displaced by Sierra Leone's
10-year civil war were also more likely to hunt illegally.
The researchers said that
collaborating across disciplines was and is key to addressing complex problems
like this one. It is commonplace for people to be noncompliant with rules and
regulations and equally important for social scientists to analyze these
behaviors.
Solomon said the team is hopeful
that the software and methodological approach will be used broadly for research
around the world.
"This is an exciting advance
that should prove useful for a variety of different topics of societal
concern," she said.