Green
energy nudges come with a hidden cost
Carnegie Mellon
University
Many US households
receive energy bills comparing their use to that of similar neighbors to remind
them to use less energy. Such policies aim to 'nudge' people toward making
better choices, both for their future selves and for others.
Nudges like these have become popular among policymakers, because they are virtually costless to implement. However, a new study finds these nudges have an unexplored cost: they can decrease support for policies with far greater impact.
Nudges like these have become popular among policymakers, because they are virtually costless to implement. However, a new study finds these nudges have an unexplored cost: they can decrease support for policies with far greater impact.
All across the United
States, many households receive energy bills comparing their use to that of
similar neighbors to remind them to use less energy. At most companies,
employees are automatically enrolled in 401(k) plans unless they choose to
opt-out, helping employees easily save for retirement.
Such policies aim to "nudge" people toward making better choices, both for their future selves and for others.
Such policies aim to "nudge" people toward making better choices, both for their future selves and for others.
Nudges like these have
become popular among policymakers, because they are virtually costless to implement.
However, a new study from researchers at Carnegie Mellon, Fordham and Harvard
universities finds that these nudges have an unexplored cost: they can decrease
support for policies with far greater impact.
"Although nudges can effectively change behavior, most have too small an impact to address societal problems on their own," said David Hagmann, a recent graduate of CMU's Department of Social and Decision Sciences, and now a postdoctoral fellow at Harvard University's Kennedy School of Government.
"It appears that many people view them as substitutes for economic policies like a carbon tax or cap-and-trade scheme, instead of the complements they were always intended to be."
Across a series of six studies, the researchers found that support for a carbon tax declines when the potential for a green energy nudge is introduced. In the first experiment, more than 70 percent of participants supported implementation of a carbon tax when it was the only option available.
However, when they could also enroll residential energy consumers into "green energy" plans by default, only 55 percent of participants favored implementing the tax.
The researchers
extended their experiments to alumni of a public policy school, to generalize
their findings to a sample of experts. Not only did they replicate their
previous findings, but a majority of those with public policy expertise and
experience believed, incorrectly, that a green energy nudge would actually be
more effective at reducing carbon emissions than a carbon tax.
"In an ideal
world, we would have a place for both nudges and heavy-handed interventions to
combat climate change," said George Loewenstein, the Herbert A. Simon
University Professor of Economics and Psychology at CMU. "However, our
results indicate that an effort to deploy nudges can backfire by reducing the
likelihood that the most effective policies will be supported and
implemented."
However, there may be
a remedy: In the final experiment presented in the paper, the researchers found
there may be a way to gain support for both a nudge and a carbon tax by
correcting perceptions at the time of decision-making.
When the researchers presented respondents with information about the small effect of a nudge or with information about how revenue from a carbon tax could be used to reduce other taxes, the nudge no longer displaced support for the carbon tax. Notably, respondents were no less likely to support the nudge after learning about its impact.
When the researchers presented respondents with information about the small effect of a nudge or with information about how revenue from a carbon tax could be used to reduce other taxes, the nudge no longer displaced support for the carbon tax. Notably, respondents were no less likely to support the nudge after learning about its impact.
"We should use
all the tools at our disposal to combat the threat of climate change,"
said Emily Ho, a doctoral student at Fordham University's Department of
Psychology.
"If we can set realistic expectations about the impact of nudges, we can deploy them without undermining the policies that are going to address climate change most effectively -- whether it's a carbon tax or a cap-and-trade system."
"If we can set realistic expectations about the impact of nudges, we can deploy them without undermining the policies that are going to address climate change most effectively -- whether it's a carbon tax or a cap-and-trade system."