Sensing food textures is a matter of pressure
Penn
State
Food's
texture affects whether it is eaten, liked or rejected, according to Penn State
researchers, who say some people are better at detecting even minor differences
in consistency because their tongues can perceive particle sizes.
That
is the key finding of a study conducted in the Sensory Evaluation Center in the
College of Agricultural Sciences by a cross-disciplinary team that included
both food and speech scientists specializing in sensory perception and
behavior.
The research included 111 volunteer tasters who had their tongues checked for physical sensitivity and then were asked their perceptions about various textures in chocolate.
The research included 111 volunteer tasters who had their tongues checked for physical sensitivity and then were asked their perceptions about various textures in chocolate.
"We've
known for a long time that individual differences in taste and smell can cause
differences in liking and food intake -- now it looks like the same might be
true for texture," said John Hayes, associate professor of food science.
"This may have implications for parents of picky eaters since texture is
often a major reason food is rejected."
The
perception of food texture arises from the interaction of a food with
mechanoreceptors in the mouth, Hayes noted. It depends on neural impulses
carried by multiple nerves.
Despite being a key driver of the acceptance or rejection of foods, he pointed out, oral texture perception remains poorly understood relative to taste and smell, two other sensory inputs critical for flavor perception.
Despite being a key driver of the acceptance or rejection of foods, he pointed out, oral texture perception remains poorly understood relative to taste and smell, two other sensory inputs critical for flavor perception.
One argument is that texture typically is not noticed when it is within an acceptable range, but that it is a major factor in rejection if an adverse texture is present, explained Hayes, director of the Sensory Evaluation Center. For chocolate specifically, oral texture is a critical quality attribute, with grittiness often being used to differentiate bulk chocolate from premium chocolates.
"Chocolate
manufacturers spend lots of energy grinding cocoa and sugar down to the right
particle size for optimal acceptability by consumers," he said. "This
work may help them figure out when it is good enough without going overboard."
Researchers
tested whether there was a relationship between oral touch sensitivity and the
perception of particle size. They used a device called Von Frey Hairs to gauge
whether participants could discriminate between different amounts of force
applied to their tongues.
When
participants were split into groups based on pressure-point sensitivity -- high
and low acuity -- there was a significant relationship between
chocolate-texture discrimination and pressure-point sensitivity for the
high-acuity group on the center tongue. However, a similar relationship was not
seen for data from the lateral edge of the tongue.
Chocolate
texture-detection experiments included both manipulated chocolates produced in
a pilot plant in the Rodney A. Erickson Food Science Building and with two
commercially produced chocolates. Because chocolate is a semi-solid suspension
of fine particles from cocoa and sugar dispersed in a continuous fat base,
Hayes explained, it is an ideal food for the study of texture.
"These
findings are novel, as we are unaware of previous work showing a relationship
between oral pressure sensitivity and ability to detect differences in particle
size in a food product," Hayes said.
"Collectively, these findings suggest that texture-detection mechanisms, which underpin point-pressure sensitivity, likely contribute to the detection of particle size in food such as chocolate."
"Collectively, these findings suggest that texture-detection mechanisms, which underpin point-pressure sensitivity, likely contribute to the detection of particle size in food such as chocolate."
Research
team member Nicole Etter, assistant professor of communication sciences and
disorders in the College of Health and Human Development, trained students on
the team to administer tactile pressure tests she developed on participants'
tongues using the Von Frey Hairs.
As a speech therapist, she explained that her interest in the findings -- recently published in Scientific Reports -- were different than the food scientists.
As a speech therapist, she explained that her interest in the findings -- recently published in Scientific Reports -- were different than the food scientists.
"The
overarching purpose of my work is to identify how we use touch sensation -- the
ability to feel our tongue move and determine where our tongue is in our mouth
-- to behave," she said. "I'm primarily interested in understanding
how a patient uses sensation from their tongue to know where and how to move
their tongue to make the proper sound."
However,
in this research, Etter said she was trying to determine whether individual
tactile sensations on the tongue relate to the ability to perceive or identify
the texture of food -- in this case, chocolate. And she focused on another
consideration, too.
"An
important aspect of speech-language pathology is helping people with feeding
and swallowing problems," she said. "Many clinical populations --
ranging from young children with disabilities to older adults with dementia --
may reject foods based on their perception of texture. This research starts to
help us understand those individual differences."
This
study sets the stage for follow-on cross-disciplinary research at Penn State,
Etter believes. She plans to collaborate with Hayes and the Sensory Evaluation
Center on studies involving foods beyond chocolate and older, perhaps
less-healthy participants to judge the ability of older people to experience
oral sensations and explore food-rejection behavior that may have serious
health and nutrition implications.
Also
involved in the research was Scott Breen, a graduate student in food science,
and Greg Ziegler, professor of food science.
Penn
State's Sensory Evaluation Center supported this work.