New research suggests gender
associations with behaviors may impact impressions, interactions
Penn State
"Rolling Coal." Would a woman do this? |
Anyone can express their commitment to the environment through individual efforts, but some pro-environmental or "green" behaviors may be seen as either feminine or masculine, which Penn State researchers say may have social consequences.
In a series of studies, the
researchers evaluated specific pro-environmental behaviors that previous
research suggested were seen as either "feminine" or
"masculine" and examined whether they affected how people were
perceived.
They found that men and women were
more likely to question a man's sexual orientation if he engaged in
"feminine" pro-environmental behaviors, such as using reusable
shopping bags.
They were also more likely to question a woman's sexual orientation if she engaged in "masculine" pro-environmental behaviors, such as caulking windows.
They were also more likely to question a woman's sexual orientation if she engaged in "masculine" pro-environmental behaviors, such as caulking windows.
Additionally, men were more likely
to avoid women who were interested in "masculine" pro-environmental
behaviors.
Janet K. Swim, professor of
psychology, said it is important to understand these social consequences
because they may hold people back from engaging in behaviors that could
ultimately help the environment.
"There may be subtle, gender-related consequences when we engage in various pro-environmental behaviors," Swim said.
"People may avoid certain behaviors because they are managing the gendered impression they anticipate others will have of them. Or they may be avoided if the behaviors they choose do not match their gender."
According to the researchers,
environmentalism in general may be seen as feminine because it fits in with
women's traditional role as caregivers. Yet, particular pro-environmental
behaviors can align with traditional feminine or masculine roles.
"Behaviors don't just help us
accomplish something concrete, they also signal something about who we
are," Swim said.
"Line drying clothes or keeping tires at proper pressures may signal that we care about the environment, but if those behaviors are seen as gendered, they may signal other things, as well."
"Line drying clothes or keeping tires at proper pressures may signal that we care about the environment, but if those behaviors are seen as gendered, they may signal other things, as well."
In three studies with a total of 960
participants, the researchers assessed impressions and avoidance of men and
women engaging in "feminine" and "masculine" behaviors.
During the first two studies,
participants read fictional summaries of a person's daily activities, which
included either feminine, masculine or neutral pro-environmental behaviors.
Participants then rated whether the person had masculine or feminine traits and guessed what the person's sexual orientation might be.
Participants then rated whether the person had masculine or feminine traits and guessed what the person's sexual orientation might be.
"Reflecting the tendency to see
environmentalism as feminine, all the people were rated as more feminine than
masculine regardless of the behaviors they did," Swim said. She also noted
that the tendency was strongest when either women or men engaged in feminine
behaviors.
The researchers found that
participants whose behaviors conformed to their gender were seen as more
heterosexual than those whose behaviors did not conform to their gender, which
may suggest participants were using traditional gender roles as clues to sexual
identity.
Additionally, the researchers
indicated that while participants did not view the nonconformists as gay or
lesbian, their ratings suggested that on average they were uncertain about
whether the person was heterosexual.
"If being seen as heterosexual
is important to a person, that person may prioritize gender-conforming over
gender-nonconforming pro-environmental behaviors in anticipation of how others
might see them," Swim said.
The researchers did a third study to
examine whether people avoided others based on the other person's
pro-environmental behavior preferences.
In a room with several other people, participants completed a digital survey where they indicted which environmental topics they would like to discuss with a partner.
In a room with several other people, participants completed a digital survey where they indicted which environmental topics they would like to discuss with a partner.
The participants were then given a
list of what they believed to be the topic preferences of four other
participants.
The list included a woman and a man who preferred discussing gender-conforming behaviors, as well as a woman and a man who preferred gender-nonconforming behaviors. The participants were then asked to rank whom they would prefer to be partnered with in order of preference.
The list included a woman and a man who preferred discussing gender-conforming behaviors, as well as a woman and a man who preferred gender-nonconforming behaviors. The participants were then asked to rank whom they would prefer to be partnered with in order of preference.
The researchers found that women
avoided men more than women, as well as people who were interested in masculine
rather than feminine behaviors. According to the researchers, although women's
partner preferences showed gender biases, these preferences did not seem to be
based on whether other's behaviors conformed to gender roles or not.
In contrast, the researchers found
that men were more likely to distance themselves from women engaging in
masculine behaviors than any of the other three potential partners. They were
equally interested in partnering with women engaging in feminine behaviors and
men who engaged in masculine or feminine behaviors.
The researchers said these results
suggested that compared to men, women were more likely to experience negative
social consequences from men for engaging in non-gender role-conforming
pro-environmental behaviors.
"We were surprised that it was
only women who experienced being avoided if they engaged in nonconforming
gender-role behaviors," Swim said. "We can't say why this is
happening, but it is a social consequence. Women may be experiencing this
negative feedback and might not know why."
The researchers said that the paper
-- recently published in the journal Sex Roles -- underlines
the importance of continuing to study gender stereotypes surrounding
environmentalism and its associated behaviors.
They added that activists and policymakers who are trying to promote pro-environmental behaviors may want to take these pressures to conform to gender roles into account as possible barriers.
They added that activists and policymakers who are trying to promote pro-environmental behaviors may want to take these pressures to conform to gender roles into account as possible barriers.
In the future, Swim said she and her
fellow researchers will continue to study the effects of pro-environmental
behaviors, such as whether social repercussions affect whether a person is willing
to do a behavior or not.
The National Science Foundation
helped support this research.