With Ritalin
and similar medications, the brain focuses on benefits instead of costs of
work, study finds
Brown University
Common assumption has long held that
Ritalin, Adderall and similar drugs work by helping people focus.
Yet a new study from a team led in
part by Brown University researchers shows that these medications
— usually prescribed to individuals diagnosed with attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder, but often used by otherwise healthy people as a “study
aid” — actually work by directing the brain to fix its attention on the
benefits, rather than the costs, of completing difficult tasks.
The study, published on
Thursday, March 19 in the journal Science, marks the first time that
scientists have examined precisely how stimulants such as Ritalin alter
cognitive function. Their research could open opportunities for further studies
to help medical professionals better understand how to identify and treat ADHD,
depression, anxiety and other mental disorders.
“People tend to think, ‘Ritalin and Adderall help me focus,’” said Michael Frank, the study’s co-senior author and a professor of cognitive, linguistic and psychological sciences at Brown.
“And they do, in some sense. But what this study shows is that they do so by increasing your cognitive motivation: Your perceived benefits of performing a demanding task are elevated, while the perceived costs are reduced. This effect is separate from any changes in actual ability.”
According to Frank, stimulants such as Ritalin increase the amount of dopamine released in the striatum, a key region in the brain related to motivation, action and cognition. Previous research has shown that dopamine, a “chemical messenger” that ferries information between neurons, can greatly influence cognitive and physical behavior.
Several past studies have found, for example, that both rodents and humans are more motivated to perform physically demanding tasks with higher dopamine.
What has remained unknown, though,
is whether dopamine can have similar motivational effects on cognition
— and that’s what a new collaborative project between Frank, Brown
postdoctoral researcher Andrew Westbrook and Dutch neuropsychiatry scholar
Roshan Cools set out to understand.
“We’ve known for a long time that
when you give people these types of stimulants, you get enhanced performance,”
said Westbrook, the study’s lead author. “But is that due to an increased
ability, or is it due to increased motivation? We didn’t know which of these
two factors were contributing and to what degree.”
Frank’s team had previously
developed mathematical
models suggesting that dopamine alters the degree to which the
striatum emphasizes the benefits, rather than the costs, of completing physical
and mental actions. Drawing upon these models, Westbrook worked with Frank and
Cools to develop an experiment that examined how stimulants that elevated
dopamine levels affected people’s mental cost-benefit analyses.
The researchers worked with 50
healthy women and men ages 18 to 43 in a lab at Radboud University in the
Netherlands. First, they measured the natural dopamine levels in each subject’s
striatum using brain imaging technology.
Then, they asked the subjects whether they would take part in a series of cognitively demanding tests, some easier and others more difficult, in exchange for certain amounts of money. Subjects who agreed to take the hardest tests stood to make the most money.
Then, they asked the subjects whether they would take part in a series of cognitively demanding tests, some easier and others more difficult, in exchange for certain amounts of money. Subjects who agreed to take the hardest tests stood to make the most money.
Each of the subjects completed the
experiment three times — once after taking a placebo; once after taking
methylphenidate, the generic version of Ritalin; and once after taking sulpiride,
an antipsychotic that elevates dopamine levels when taken in low doses and is
often used to treat symptoms of schizophrenia and major depressive disorder at
much higher doses. The researchers used a double-blind experiment design, where
neither they nor the subjects knew which pill was being given to each subject.
The results largely matched
Westbrook’s computer-modeled predictions. Those with lower dopamine levels made
decisions that indicated they were more focused on avoiding difficult cognitive
work — in other words, they were more sensitive to the potential costs of
completing the task.
Those with higher dopamine levels, on the other hand, made decisions that showed they were more sensitive to differences in the amount of money they could earn by choosing the harder test — in other words, they focused more on the potential benefits.
Westbrook said the latter held true whether the subjects’ dopamine levels were naturally higher or whether they had been artificially elevated by medications.
Those with higher dopamine levels, on the other hand, made decisions that showed they were more sensitive to differences in the amount of money they could earn by choosing the harder test — in other words, they focused more on the potential benefits.
Westbrook said the latter held true whether the subjects’ dopamine levels were naturally higher or whether they had been artificially elevated by medications.
Westbrook said the results support
the idea that, medication or no medication, dopamine typically acts as a
motivation regulator for human brains.
“The thoughts that pop into our
head, and the amount of time we spend thinking about them, are regulated by
this underlying cost-benefit decision-making system,” Westbrook said. “Our
brains have been honed to orient us toward the tasks that will have the
greatest payoff and the least cost over time.”
All of us have slightly different
base levels of dopamine, said Frank, who is affiliated with the Carney
Institute for Brain Science at Brown. Those who have lower levels tend to be
more risk-averse, because they spend more time focusing on the potential costs
of completing a difficult task. Those with higher levels tend to be more
impulsive and active, because they focus more on the benefits.
No single dopamine level is inherently better than another, Frank said — an active, high-dopamine person may take fulfilling, happiness-boosting risks but may also be more prone to injury; a risk-averse, low-dopamine person may avoid injuries and disappointments but may also miss out on adventures.
And dopamine levels don’t necessarily stay the same from one day to the next: They may decrease in response to danger or lack of sleep, and they may increase when people feel safe and supported.
In other words, Westbrook said, most
people can trust natural dopamine levels to guide them toward the right
decisions. Of course, previous experiments have made it clear that many people
with particularly low dopamine levels — including those who are diagnosed with
depression or ADHD — can benefit from dopamine-boosting stimulant
medications.
But he said those medications are never certain to improve the lives of those who are healthy and who choose to use them recreationally. Doing so could, in fact, lead some to make poorer decisions.
But he said those medications are never certain to improve the lives of those who are healthy and who choose to use them recreationally. Doing so could, in fact, lead some to make poorer decisions.
“When you raise dopamine in someone
who already has a high dopamine level, every decision seems like it has
benefit, which could distract from the real beneficial tasks,” Westbrook said.
“People might behave in ways that aren’t consistent with their goals, like
taking part in impulsive gambling or risky sex behaviors.”
Westbrook and Frank hope their study
helps future researchers and medical professionals better understand cognitive
mechanisms, allowing them to identify connections between dopamine levels and
disorders such as anxiety, depression, ADHD and schizophrenia.
“We want to know, what are the
drivers of what changes cognitive ability and function?” Frank said. “Our
research is focused on carving nature at its joints, so to speak —
disentangling neural and cognitive functions to understand people’s different
thought processes and evaluate what’s best for their needs, whether it’s
therapy or medication.”
Also involved in the study were Radboud University researchers Ruben van den Bosch, Jessica Määttä, Lieke Hofmans and Danae Papadopetraki. The research was funded by a Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research VICI grant, No. 453-14-005 (2015/01379/VI) and two National Institutes of Health grants (F32MH115600-01A1 and R01MH080066).
Also involved in the study were Radboud University researchers Ruben van den Bosch, Jessica Määttä, Lieke Hofmans and Danae Papadopetraki. The research was funded by a Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research VICI grant, No. 453-14-005 (2015/01379/VI) and two National Institutes of Health grants (F32MH115600-01A1 and R01MH080066).