Brain waves can point the way
University
of California - Berkeley
Anyone who has tried and failed to meditate knows that our minds are rarely still. But where do they roam?
New research led by UC Berkeley has come up with a way to
track the flow of our internal thought processes and signal whether our minds
are focused, fixated or wandering.
Using
an electroencephalogram (EEG) to measure brain activity while people performed
mundane attention tasks, researchers identified brain signals that reveal when
the mind is not focused on the task at hand or aimlessly wandering, especially
after concentrating on an assignment.
Specifically,
increased alpha brain waves were detected in the prefrontal cortex of more than
two dozen study participants when their thoughts jumped from one topic to
another, providing an electrophysiological signature for unconstrained,
spontaneous thought. Alpha waves are slow brain rhythms whose frequency ranges
from 9 to 14 cycles per second.
Meanwhile,
weaker brain signals known as P3 were observed in the parietal cortex, further
offering a neural marker for when people are not paying attention to the task
at hand.
"For the first time, we have neurophysiological evidence that distinguishes different patterns of internal thought, allowing us to understand the varieties of thought central to human cognition and to compare between healthy and disordered thinking," said study senior author Robert Knight, a UC Berkeley professor of psychology and neuroscience.
The
findings, published this week in the Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences journal, suggest that tuning out our external
environment and allowing our internal thoughts to move freely and creatively
are a necessary function of the brain and can promote relaxation and exploration.
Moreover,
EEG markers of how our thoughts flow when our brains are at rest can help
researchers and clinicians detect certain patterns of thinking, even before
patients are aware of where their minds are wandering.
"This
could help detect thought patterns linked to a spectrum of psychiatric and
attention disorders and may help diagnose them," said study lead author
Julia Kam, an assistant professor of psychology at the University of Calgary.
She launched the study as a postdoctoral researcher in Knight's cognitive
neuroscience lab at UC Berkeley.
Another
co-author on the paper is Zachary Irving, an assistant professor of philosophy
at the University of Virginia who explored the psychological and philosophical
underpinnings of mind-wandering as a postdoctoral scholar at UC Berkeley.
"If
you focus all the time on your goals, you can miss important information. And
so, having a free-association thought process that randomly generates memories
and imaginative experiences can lead you to new ideas and insights," said
Irving, whose philosophical theory of mind-wandering shaped the study's
methodology.
Irving
worked with Alison Gopnik, a UC Berkeley developmental psychologist and
philosophy scholar who is also a co-author of the study.
"Babies
and young children's minds seem to wander constantly, and so we wondered what
functions that might serve," Gopnik said. "Our paper suggests
mind-wandering is as much a positive feature of cognition as a quirk and
explains something we all experience."
To
prepare for the study, 39 adults were taught the difference between four
different categories of thinking: task-related, freely moving, deliberately
constrained and automatically constrained.
Next,
while wearing electrodes on their heads that measured their brain activity,
they sat at a computer screen and tapped left or right arrow keys to correspond
with left and right arrows appearing in random sequences on the screen.
When
they finished a sequence, they were asked to rate on a scale of one to seven --
whether their thoughts during the task had been related to the task, freely
moving, deliberately constrained or automatically constrained.
One
example of thoughts unrelated to the task and freely moving would be if a
student, instead of studying for an upcoming exam, found herself thinking about
whether she had received a good grade on an assignment, then realized she had
not yet prepared dinner, and then wondered if she should exercise more, and
ended up reminiscing about her last vacation, Kam said.
The
responses to the questions about thought processes were then divided into the
four groups and matched against the recorded brain activity.
When
study participants reported having thoughts that moved freely from topic to
topic, they showed increased alpha wave activity in the brain's frontal cortex,
a pattern linked to the generation of creative ideas. Researchers also found
evidence of lesser P3 brain signals during off-task thoughts.
"The
ability to detect our thought patterns through brain activity is an important
step toward generating potential strategies for regulating how our thoughts
unfold over time, a strategy useful for healthy and disordered minds
alike," Kam said.
In
addition to Kam, Knight, Irving and Gopnik, co-authors of the study are Shawn
Patel at UC Berkeley and Caitlin Mills at the University of Hampshire.