Studying the radical mind
University
of Cambridge
Researchers have mapped an underlying "psychological signature" for people who are predisposed to holding extreme social, political or religious attitudes, and support violence in the name of ideology.
A
new study suggests that a particular mix of personality traits and unconscious
cognition -- the ways our brains take in basic information -- is a strong predictor
for extremist views across a range of beliefs, including nationalism and
religious fervour.
These
mental characteristics include poorer working memory and slower
"perceptual strategies" -- the unconscious processing of changing
stimuli, such as shape and colour -- as well as tendencies towards impulsivity
and sensation seeking.
This
combination of cognitive and emotional attributes predicts the endorsement of
violence in support of a person's ideological "group," according to
findings published today in Philosophical Transactions of the Royal
Society B.
The
study also maps the psychological signatures that underpin fierce political
conservatism, as well as "dogmatism": people who have a fixed
worldview and are resistant to evidence.
Psychologists
found that conservatism is linked to cognitive "caution":
slow-and-accurate unconscious decision-making, compared to the
fast-and-imprecise "perceptual strategies" found in more liberal
minds.
Brains
of more dogmatic people are slower to process perceptual evidence, but they are
more impulsive personality-wise. The mental signature for extremism across the board
is a blend of conservative and dogmatic psychologies.
Researchers from the University of Cambridge say that, while still in early stages, this research could help to better identify and support people most vulnerable to radicalisation across the political and religious spectrum.
Approaches
to radicalisation policy mainly rely on basic demographic information such as
age, race and gender. By adding cognitive and personality assessments, the
psychologists created a statistical model that is between four and fifteen
times more powerful at predicting ideological worldviews than demographics
alone.
"I'm
interested in the role that hidden cognitive functions play in sculpting
ideological thinking," said Dr Leor Zmigrod, lead author from Cambridge's
Department of Psychology.
"Many
people will know those in their communities who have become radicalised or
adopted increasingly extreme political views, whether on the left or right. We
want to know why particular individuals are more susceptible."
"By
examining 'hot' emotional cognition alongside the 'cold' unconscious cognition
of basic information processing we can see a psychological signature for those
at risk of engaging with an ideology in an extreme way," Zmigrod said.
"Subtle
difficulties with complex mental processing may subconsciously push people
towards extreme doctrines that provide clearer, more defined explanations of
the world, making them susceptible to toxic forms of dogmatic and authoritarian
ideologies."
The
research is published as part of a special issue of the Royal Society journal
dedicated to "the political brain" compiled and co-edited by Zmigrod.
It
is the latest in a series of studies by Zmigrod investigating the relationship
between ideology and cognition. She has previously published findings on links
between cognitive "inflexibility" and religious extremism,
willingness to self-sacrifice for a cause, and a vote for Brexit.
A
2019 study by Zmigrod showed that this cognitive inflexibility is found in
those with extreme attitudes on both the far right and far left of the
political divide.
The
latest research builds on work from Stanford University in which hundreds of
study participants performed 37 different cognitive tasks and took 22 different
personality surveys in 2016 and 2017.
Zmigrod
and colleagues, including Cambridge psychologist Professor Trevor Robbins,
conducted a series of follow-up tests in 2018 on 334 of the original
participants, using a further 16 surveys to determine attitudes and strength of
feeling towards various ideologies.
Political
conservatism and nationalism was related to "caution" in unconscious
decision-making, as well as "temporal discounting" -- when rewards
are seen to lose value if delayed -- and slightly reduced strategic information
processing in the cognitive domain.
Personality
traits for conservatism and nationalism included greater goal-directedness,
impulsivity and reward sensitivity, and reduced social risk-taking.
Demographics alone had a predictive power of less than 8% for these ideologies,
but adding the psychological signature boosted it to 32.5%.
Dogmatism
was linked to reduced speed of perceptual "evidence accumulation,"
and reduced social risk-taking and agreeableness but heightened impulsivity and
ethical risk-taking in the personality domain. Religiosity was cognitively
similar to conservatism, but with higher levels of agreeableness and "risk
perception."
Adding
the psychological signatures to demographics increased the predictive power for
dogmatism from 1.53% to 23.6%, and religiosity from 2.9% to 23.4%.
Across
all ideologies investigated by the researchers, people who endorsed
"extreme pro-group action," including ideologically-motivated
violence against others, had a surprisingly consistent psychological profile.
The
extremist mind -- a mixture of conservative and dogmatic psychological
signatures -- is cognitively cautious, slower at perceptual processing and has
a weaker working memory. This is combined with impulsive personality traits
that seek sensation and risky experiences.
Added
Zmigrod: "There appear to be hidden similarities in the minds of those
most willing to take extreme measures to support their ideological doctrines.
Understanding this could help us to support those individuals vulnerable to
extremism, and foster social understanding across ideological divides."
Study
participants were all from the United States, 49.4% were female, and ages
ranged from 22-63.
Part
of the study used tests of "executive functions" that help us to
plan, organise and execute tasks e.g. restacking coloured disks to match
guidelines, and keeping a series of categorised words in mind as new ones are
added.
Additionally,
results from various rapid decision-making tests -- switching between visual
stimuli based on evolving instructions, for example -- were fed into
computational models, allowing analyses of small differences in perceptual
processing.
Researchers
took the results of the in-depth, self-reported personality tests and boiled
them down to 12 key factors ranging from goal-directedness and emotional
control to financial risk-taking.
The
examination of social and political attitudes took in a host of ideological
positions including patriotism, religiosity and levels of authoritarianism on
the left and right.
The
Cambridge team used data modeling techniques such as Bayesian analyses to
extract correlations. They then measured the extent to which blends of
cognition and personality could help predict ideological attitudes.