Negative stories without context can undermine confidence in science
Annenberg Public Policy Center of the University of Pennsylvania
News
media reports about scientific failures that do not recognize the
self-correcting nature of science can damage public perceptions of trust and
confidence in scientific work, according to findings by researchers at the
Annenberg Public Policy Center (APPC) of the University of Pennsylvania and the
University at Buffalo, the State University of New York.How the scientific method works
News stories about science follow several specific narratives, the researchers write in a new study in the journal Public Understanding of Science.
One is that science is "in crisis" or
"broken," a narrative driven in recent years by reports of
unsuccessful efforts to replicate findings in psychology, a rise in
retractions, failures of peer review, and the misuse of statistics, among other
things.
"Attempts and failures to replicate findings are an essential and healthy part of the scientific process," said co-author Yotam Ophir, an assistant professor of communication at the University of Buffalo and a former postdoctoral fellow in APPC's science of science communication program, where the work was conducted.
"Our research shows the need for journalists and scientists to accurately
contextualize such failures as part of the self-correcting nature of
science."
In an experiment, nearly 4,500 U.S. adults were assigned to read one of four different types of news stories about science or a control story. Among the findings:
- Exposure to stories highlighting problems reduced trust in scientists and induced negative beliefs about scientists.
- Greater effects were seen among people who read stories saying that science was in crisis or broken.
"We've
identified a tendency in news coverage to overgeneralize the prevalence of
problems in science and take them as an indicator that the enterprise as a
whole is broken," said co-author and APPC Director Kathleen Hall Jamieson.
What the experiment found, she added, is that "exposure to news that
mistakenly concluded that because something has gone wrong science is in crisis
can unjustifiably undercut confidence in science."
The experiment
The study sought
to provide experimental evidence about the effects of exposure to different
narratives about science. It was conducted online with 4,497 U.S. adults in
early 2019 -- before, Jamieson noted, the world was in the throes of the
Covid-19 pandemic and "science discovered life-saving vaccines with
unprecedented speed."
The experiment
tested the effects of four narratives:
- the "honorable quest" or discovery, in which a scientist discovers knowledge that is reliable and consequential;
- the "counterfeit quest," or retraction of published work, in which a scientist engages in dishonorable and guileful conduct;
- the science is "in crisis/broken" narrative, which indicts scientists or the institution of science for failing to address a known problem; and the "problem explored," where scientists explore and potentially fix a problem revealed by the "crisis/broken" narrative.
Participants were randomly assigned a reading based on edited news stories that were consistent with one of the narratives. For example, one "quest" story told of a discovery in immunotherapy to treat leukemia, while a "counterfeit quest" story described retracted scientific claims about eating behavior.
A "science is broken" story described an "alarming
increase in the number of retractions," and a "problem explored"
story looked at psychologists exploring ways to increase the reliability of
psychology studies. A fifth group of participants read a control story about an
unrelated subject, baseball.
After completing
the readings, the participants were asked about their trust in science, beliefs
about science, and support for funding of science.
Trust in science is high
The researchers
found that:
- Trust in science was moderately high;
- Beliefs that science is self-correcting and beneficial were moderate to high;
- Among people with higher levels of trust in science, the more they perceived the problem-focused stories to be representative of science, the more likely they were to believe that science is self-correcting;
- For people with lower levels of trust in science, the effect was reversed: the more they saw the problem-focused stories as representative, the less likely they were to believe that science is self-correcting;
- Support for funding science was not affected by the stories.
"This study,"
the authors concluded, "demonstrates the adverse, if small, effects of
problem-focused media narratives on trust in, beliefs about, and support for
scientists and points to the importance of perceived representativeness and
audience trust in scientists in the audience's response to them."
The experiment
follows up on a 2018 study by Jamieson in the Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences . The earlier study examined three media narratives about
science -- the honorable quest, counterfeit quest, and crisis/broken. Of the
crisis/broken articles examined in that study, just 29% indicated that science
is self-correcting and 34% were written by a scientist. That study expressed
concern that "defective narratives can enhance the capacity of partisans
to discredit areas of science... containing findings that are ideologically
uncongenial to them."
How journalists and scientists can bolster trust in
science
"By
labeling problems in scientific research 'a crisis' and by framing scientific
failures as indications that science is unreliable, both scientists and
journalists are failing to communicate the true values of science," Ophir
said. "Making mistakes is part of science. What the news media and
scientists themselves often frame as failure is an indicator of healthy
science."
The content
analysis found that honorable quest story was the most prevalent. But the study
noted that when media reports do discuss failures "they tend to ignore
scientific attempts to address the problems," the authors write. "We
argue that such narratives about individual or systemic scientific failures
fail to communicate scientific norms of continuing exploration, scrutiny, and
skepticism and could, particularly if being presented regularly and
consistently, harm public trust and confidence in scientific work."
Use of the "problem explored" narrative could lessen the detrimental effects and improve attitudes toward science by "better communicating scientific norms of continuing exploration, scrutiny, and skepticism," the authors write.
"As scientific communication in news media is the result of a negotiation
between scientists and journalists, these results could guide future science
communication efforts by both journalists and members of the scientific
community.
"Like
others before us..." they conclude, "we believe that such a change
will require scientific institutions to reconsider the current incentive
structure, that prioritizes the promotion of novel, statistically significant
discoveries over [rigorous] self-correction efforts."
The Annenberg
Public Policy Center was established in 1993 to educate the public and policy
makers about communication's role in advancing public understanding of
political, science, and health issues at the local, state, and federal levels.