Powerful people are less likely to be understanding when mistakes are made
University of California - San Diego
Those with power, such as the wealthy are more likely to blame others for having shortcomings and they are also less troubled by reports of inequality, according to recent research from the University of California San Diego's Rady School of Management.
The study published in Social Psychological and Personality Science defines power as control over valuable resources.
The paper finds that people in positions of power are more likely to adopt a "choice-mindset," which means that although they have more choices (the definition of power in many cases) they still see others with less power as having lots of choice, regardless of their situation.
Consequently, high-power individuals are more
likely to blame others if they perform poorly and they are also more likely to
punish them.
"Being in a choice-mindset changes how individuals think, feel and behave," said Yidan Yin, the first author of the paper and recent PhD graduate from the Rady School. "Compared to low-power people, high-power people are less likely to be aware of others' constraints. As a result they assign more blame when people make mistakes or have shortcomings. Thus, they see the current hierarchy as more justified."
The
results from the research were derived from three different studies the authors
conducted to replicate the findings in different settings.
Research
findings hold up in three robust study settings
The first study was conducted via a survey in which the researchers measured the sense of power of 363 members of the general public recruited through the platform Prolific. They also asked the participants to complete a separate survey, claiming it was unrelated, in which they were crowdsourcing how to resolve a human resources issue at the university.
The survey explained that
their academic department was weighing whether to give an administrative
assistant a bonus though they had missed a deadline on a big project due to
conflicting priorities. The survey participants who measured as having a
greater sense of power overwhelming responded the administrative assistant did
not deserve the bonus and that their excuses were without merit.
The
second study was conducted with the platform Amazon Mechanical Turk involving
393 members of the general public who were randomly assigned to roles as supervisors
and subordinates in completing various tasks. Though the assigned ranks were
random, supervisors were told they earned the role for their proficiencies and
subordinates were told they were designated as such because they were
outperformed by supervisors.
The
two groups had to judge the performance of an anonymous individual, who by
design, made mistakes in completing their tasks. Once again, the researchers
found that those with a greater sense of power (supervisors) were harsher, less
understanding with their judgment and recommended punishment more than
subordinates.
The
third study was done in a lab with UC San Diego undergraduates and mirrored the
second experiment. The main difference was that both supervisors and
subordinates knew that the target person they had to judge had the rank of a
subordinate and therefore less choices. The results from the first two studies
held up with subjects that had more power assigning more blame and recommending
more punishment.
"Each
study was designed to build on the others," Yidan and co-author Pamela K.
Smith, associate professor of economics and strategic management at the Rady
School write. "In study one, we were measuring power, in study two, we
manipulated power and in study three, we created a world in which the judges
knew the target person had less power and less choices. We wanted to see if the
perceptions remained consistent in all three settings. It was a combination of
replication and adding these additional twists and turns."
Implications
for more equitable public policy and workplace environments
The
results from the study have significant implications for public policy,
according to the authors.
"Policymakers are in a position of power and privilege and may be less sensitive to the disadvantages of their constituents," the authors write.
"This is
especially important as we come out of the pandemic when there are big
discussions in the political domain on pulling back on unemployment benefits,
or rent assistance. If you are in a position of power, you may assume people
are choosing to stay home and not work and they can make better choices.
However, you may need to think much more carefully about how many choices
citizens have and if you are missing constraints they face."
In
addition, the implications are far-reaching for the workplace.
"Mangers
should be aware of how many more choices they have than their subordinates and
their tendency to project their own choices onto others, especially when
employees make mistakes." Yin said.
Smith
added, "It might require having more discussion with employees and being
cognizant of their situation because sometimes lack of choice and constraints
can be invisible to someone from the outside."