By Anne Manning
When a hurricane threatens to make landfall, forecasters offer a barrage of informational tools to communicate the risk of it coming through coastal and inland communities, so residents can prepare for its impact.
Chief among these tools is the “cone of uncertainty” – a visual depiction of the storm’s potential path.
But is the cone doing its job?
But is the cone doing its job?
Studies show that people often misinterpret this popular weather graphic. They don’t understand the information it’s conveying: the likely path of a storm, and its likelihood to deviate from that path based on historical data.
The graphic is cone-shaped because the farther we try to look into the future, the more uncertain the forecast. But because the cone draws a line around a specific area, many people assume that locations outside the cone will not be affected by the storm.
Researchers in Colorado State University’s Department of Psychology are working on an easily understood, science-backed way to visually represent hurricane danger to the general public. They contend that the cone of uncertainty creates a false sense of security for people who live outside the boundary of the cone and that there are better ways to signal likely impacts.
The research team includes psychology professors Jessica Witt, who studies the human visual system, and Benjamin Clegg, who studies human factors in the design of new technologies. Together, they created experiments to test whether hurricane projections could be better understood by average viewers through dynamic graphics the researchers have christened “zoomies.” Their results are detailed in Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied.
Summarizing information
According to Witt, the human visual system excels at something called ensemble perception. When your eyes see a group of objects, your brain quickly extracts a summary of those objects. Looking at a patch of grass, your brain makes a snap judgement about its average greenness. When you look at a tree, your brain automatically estimates the average size of the leaves.
The cone of uncertainty is what researchers call a summary statistic. The average, projected track of the hurricane goes up the middle, and that track is surrounded by varying degrees of uncertainty.
When the cone gets bigger, people think that means the storm will be getting worse or increasing in severity. But the cone’s size is only communicating increasing uncertainty around the forecast. The cone also lends itself to what researchers call a containment heuristic.
“People like categories, and to be able to put things in these binary buckets – [at] risk, not at risk,” Witt said. “The cone basically encourages that. It has this well-defined boundary, and people treat things within a boundary as qualitatively different than what’s outside the boundary.”
The researchers wondered if instead of the summary statistic of the cone, a better graphic would take advantage of what the visual system is already good at – synthesizing and summarizing. “Rather than visualize the summary, let’s give them raw data, and let the visual system do the summary instead,” Witt said.
‘Zoomies’
Their new and improved graphic is more like a track ensemble, or a spaghetti plot. But track ensembles also have their issues. If a town is located on a track, then people perceive it to be at higher risk than one located off a track, even if the latter one is located closer to the storm’s center.
So Witt and Clegg came up with the idea of “zoomies,” which are sets of dots that each represent a different projected hurricane path and move accordingly. “The idea is that by getting rid of the defined boundary, we do not have this yes-or-no binary risk distinction, but rather a more gradual, more probabilistic understanding of risk,” Witt said.
Lots of zoomies following paths close to the most likely path convey the higher risk there. But even a few zoomies showing more extreme deviations illustrate that there is still some risk for those areas, the researchers said.
Their hypothesis was borne out in a series of experiments with CSU students who, the researchers noted in their paper, are typically not very experienced with hurricanes. In the experiment, they tasked participants with deciding whether to evacuate a town on a map, based on seeing either a traditional cone of uncertainty or the experimental zoomies.
The research team includes psychology professors Jessica Witt, who studies the human visual system, and Benjamin Clegg, who studies human factors in the design of new technologies. Together, they created experiments to test whether hurricane projections could be better understood by average viewers through dynamic graphics the researchers have christened “zoomies.” Their results are detailed in Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied.
Summarizing information
According to Witt, the human visual system excels at something called ensemble perception. When your eyes see a group of objects, your brain quickly extracts a summary of those objects. Looking at a patch of grass, your brain makes a snap judgement about its average greenness. When you look at a tree, your brain automatically estimates the average size of the leaves.
The cone of uncertainty is what researchers call a summary statistic. The average, projected track of the hurricane goes up the middle, and that track is surrounded by varying degrees of uncertainty.
When the cone gets bigger, people think that means the storm will be getting worse or increasing in severity. But the cone’s size is only communicating increasing uncertainty around the forecast. The cone also lends itself to what researchers call a containment heuristic.
“People like categories, and to be able to put things in these binary buckets – [at] risk, not at risk,” Witt said. “The cone basically encourages that. It has this well-defined boundary, and people treat things within a boundary as qualitatively different than what’s outside the boundary.”
The researchers wondered if instead of the summary statistic of the cone, a better graphic would take advantage of what the visual system is already good at – synthesizing and summarizing. “Rather than visualize the summary, let’s give them raw data, and let the visual system do the summary instead,” Witt said.
‘Zoomies’
Their new and improved graphic is more like a track ensemble, or a spaghetti plot. But track ensembles also have their issues. If a town is located on a track, then people perceive it to be at higher risk than one located off a track, even if the latter one is located closer to the storm’s center.
So Witt and Clegg came up with the idea of “zoomies,” which are sets of dots that each represent a different projected hurricane path and move accordingly. “The idea is that by getting rid of the defined boundary, we do not have this yes-or-no binary risk distinction, but rather a more gradual, more probabilistic understanding of risk,” Witt said.
Lots of zoomies following paths close to the most likely path convey the higher risk there. But even a few zoomies showing more extreme deviations illustrate that there is still some risk for those areas, the researchers said.
Their hypothesis was borne out in a series of experiments with CSU students who, the researchers noted in their paper, are typically not very experienced with hurricanes. In the experiment, they tasked participants with deciding whether to evacuate a town on a map, based on seeing either a traditional cone of uncertainty or the experimental zoomies.