Research focuses on what dieters will pay to avoid temptation
New York University
A team of researchers has identified measures we take in order to skirt having to exert self-control in our daily lives -- findings that offer new insights into what motivates human behavior.
"The failure to use self-control is a problem that humans face every day, but we know little about the cognitive processes that lie beneath it or the lengths we'll go to avoid needing to implement it," explains Candace Raio, an assistant professor of research in the Department of Psychiatry at NYU Grossman School of Medicine and the lead author of the paper, which appears in the journal Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (PNAS).
"In this study, we
were able to determine the price people are willing to pay to eliminate the
need for self-control and measure how it changes under different levels of
temptation, motivational incentives, and exposure to stress."
"Our approach allows us to 'index' individuals' moment-to-moment 'self-control costs', offering a means to inform psychological, economic, and health-policy research," adds senior author Paul Glimcher, a professor in the Neuroscience Institute and in the Department of Psychiatry at NYU Langone Health and director NYU's Institute for the Study of Decision Making.
Scientists have long argued that the use of self-control requires significant psychological effort and that its failure occurs when the cognitive costs of self-control outweigh its perceived benefits.
However, until now that has been mostly an intuitive, rather than a scientific, explanation. Our lack of an empirical tool to quantify and demonstrate the actual mental cost of self-control has limited economic and health-policy research in a fundamental way.
To address this,
the researchers conducted a series of experiments that precisely quantified how
much dieters would pay to avoid being exposed to a tempting food reward under
different circumstances -- with the aim of establishing a measurement for the
"cost of self-control."
In these experiments, participants reported the most they were willing to pay from a $10 endowment to remove a tempting food reward -- for example a chocolate brownie -- which would be placed in front of them for 30 minutes.
By examining
participants under a range of conditions, the researchers were also able to
demonstrate that the cost of self-control increased when subjects were stressed
and when they were offered motivational incentives (i.e., a monetary bonus) to
avoid eating the tempting food. In both conditions, participants paid more to
have temptation removed, a finding with striking implications for the economic
costs of stress on behavioral control.
Overall, the
researchers found that the more tempting the food item, determined by
participants' own ratings, the more these participants were willing to pay to
avoid having to exert self-control, suggesting these costs reflect
participants' aversion to resisting temptation.
Across these
five experiments, Raio and Glimcher were able to index the real economic costs
of self-control, in dollars and cents, and show how they relate to real-world
temptations.
The research was
supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (F32MH110135,
R01DA038063) and an NARSAD Young Investigator Grant from the Brain &
Behavior Research Foundation.