Playlist is switched up and remembered for at least 30 minutes
Duke University
The tweets of a little song sparrow and its 'bird brain' are a lot more complex and akin to human language than anyone realized.
A new study finds that male
sparrows deliberately shuffle and mix their song repertoire possibly as a way
to keep it interesting for their female audience.
The
research, from the lab of Stephen Nowicki, Duke University professor of biology
and member of the Duke Institute for Brain Sciences, and colleagues at the
University of Miami, shows that singing males keep track of the order of their
songs and how often each one is sung for up to 30 minutes so they can curate
both their current playlist and the next one. The findings appear in Proceedings
of the Royal Society B on January 26.
Song
sparrows are a common songbird throughout North America, but only males sing.
They use their song to defend their turf and court mates.
When wooing, song sparrows belt up to 12 different two-second songs, a repertoire that can take nearly 30 minutes to get through, since they repeat the same song several times before going on to the next track. In addition to varying the number of repeats, males also shuffle the order of their tunes each time they sing their discography. However, a big unknown had been whether males change up their song order and repeats by accident or by design.
To
get some data on whether or not the birds intentionally shuffle and mix their
tunes, Nowicki's long-time collaborator William Searcy, the Maytag Professor of
Ornithology in Biology at the University of Miami, loaded up the recording
gear, trekked out to the backwoods of northwest Pennsylvania, set up mics
pointed to the trees and patiently waited for five hours a day.
Nowicki
says that fieldwork like this isn't for everyone, "I would never use the
word boring, because it's relaxing if you like being out in the field and it's
a nice day and you've got your parabolic microphone and you're pointing it at a
song sparrow for hours. Some people would find that boring. I and certainly
Bill would find that meditatively relaxing. The only thing that happens is sometimes
your arm gets tired."
After
recording the full suite of songs from more than 30 birds, the team pored over
visual spectrographs of the trills and analyzed how often each song was sung
and in what order. The first clue that males keep tabs on their tweets to avoid
repetition was that much like a Spotify playlist, males generally sing through
their full repertoire before repeating a song.
The
researchers also found that the more a sparrow sang a given song, the longer he
took to get back to that song, possibly to build up hype and novelty once that
song was played again. For example, if a male sang Song A 10 times in a row,
he'd sing even more renditions of his other songs before returning to Song A
again. Alternatively, if Song A was only warbled three times during a set, then
a male song sparrow might recite a shorter rendition of the rest of his
repertoire in order to return to the still novel and underplayed Song A.
Taken
together, these findings demonstrate that song sparrows possess an extremely rare
talent with an equally uncommon name: "long-distance dependencies."
It means that what a male song sparrow sings in the moment depends on what he
sang as much as 30 minutes ago. That's a 360 times larger memory capacity than
the previous record holder, the canary, who can only juggle about five seconds
worth of song information in this way.
While
impressive, the implications from this work for humans are less clear. It does
suggest that the order of words in human language, which is similarly impacted by
long-distance dependencies may not be as unique as once thought.
It
remains to be seen whether better shuffling ability gives males an advantage at
finding love. Perhaps females maintain interest in a mate who mixes it up more,
and are less likely to sneak off with another male. As with daytime talk shows,
paternity tests are a good proxy for monogamy in birds, so counting how many
chicks are sired by a female's nest mate versus another bird in the
neighborhood may be a future project for Nowicki's team.
For
now, Nowicki emphasizes it's just speculation whether these shuffling song
sparrows give Spotify a run for their money to keep a female's interest, but
does highlight our similar approach at the gym.
"You've
got your playlist for running and the reason you've got that is because running
is kind of boring. You know that these 10 songs are going to keep you
motivated, but if you are going to run for 20 songs long, why not shuffle it so
the next time you don't hear the same songs in the same order?"
Support
for the research came from the Office of the Provost, Duke University, and the
Robert E. Maytag Professor Research Fund of the University of Miami.