Flies possess more sophisticated cognitive abilities than previously known
University of California - San Diego
As they annoyingly buzz around a batch of bananas in our kitchens, fruit flies appear to have little in common with mammals. But as a model species for science, researchers are discovering increasing similarities between us and the miniscule fruit-loving insects.
In
a new study, researchers at the University of California San Diego's Kavli
Institute for Brain and Mind (KIBM) have found that fruit flies (Drosophila
melanogaster) have more advanced cognitive abilities than previously
believed. Using a custom-built immersive virtual reality environment,
neurogenetic manipulations and in vivo real-time
brain-activity imaging, the scientists present new evidence Feb. 16 in the
journal Nature of the remarkable links between the cognitive
abilities of flies and mammals.
The
multi-tiered approach of their investigations found attention, working memory
and conscious awareness-like capabilities in fruit flies, cognitive abilities
typically only tested in mammals. The researchers were able to watch the
formation, distractibility and eventual fading of a memory trace in their tiny
brains.
"Despite a lack of obvious anatomical similarity, this research speaks to our everyday cognitive functioning -- what we pay attention to and how we do it," said study senior author Ralph Greenspan, a professor in the UC San Diego Division of Biological Sciences and associate director of KIBM. "Since all brains evolved from a common ancestor, we can draw correspondences between fly and mammalian brain regions based on molecular characteristics and how we store our memories."
To
arrive at the heart of their new findings the researchers created an immersive
virtual reality environment to test the fly's behavior via visual stimulation
and coupled the displayed imagery with an infra-red laser as an averse heat
stimulus. The near 360-degree panoramic arena allowed Drosophila to
flap their wings freely while remaining tethered, and with the virtual reality
constantly updating based on their wing movement (analyzed in real-time using
high-speed machine-vision cameras) it gave the flies the illusion of flying
freely in the world. This gave researchers the ability to train and test flies
for conditioning tasks by allowing the insect to orient away from an image
associated with the negative heat stimulus and towards a second image not
associated with heat.
They
tested two variants of conditioning, one in which flies were given visual
stimulation overlapping in time with the heat (delay conditioning), both ending
together, or a second, trace conditioning, by waiting 5 to 20 seconds to
deliver the heat after showing and removing the visual stimulation. The
intervening time is considered the "trace" interval during which the
fly retains a "trace" of the visual stimulus in its brain, a feature
indicative of attention, working memory and conscious awareness in mammals.
The
researchers also imaged the brain to track calcium activity in real-time using
a fluorescent molecule they genetically engineered into their brain cells. This
allowed the researchers to record the formation and duration of the fly's
living memory since they saw the trace blinking on and off while being held in
the fly's short-term (working) memory. They also found that a distraction
introduced during training -- a gentle puff of air -- made the visual memory
fade more quickly, marking the first time researchers have been able to prove
such distractedness in flies and implicating an attentional requirement in
memory formation in Drosophila.
"This
work demonstrates not only that flies are capable of this higher form of trace
conditioning, and that the learning is distractible just like in mammals and
humans, but the neural activity underlying these attentional and working memory
processes in the fly show remarkable similarity to those in mammals," said
Dhruv Grover, a UC San Diego KIBM research faculty member and lead author of
the new study. "This work demonstrates that fruit flies could serve as a
powerful model for the study of higher cognitive functions. Simply put, the fly
continues to amaze in how smart it really is."
The
scientists also identified the area of the fly's brain where the memory formed
and faded -- an area known as the ellipsoid body of the fly's central complex,
a location that corresponds to the cerebral cortex in the human brain.
Further,
the research team discovered that the neurochemical dopamine is required for
such learning and higher cognitive functions. The data revealed that dopamine
reactions increasingly occurred earlier in the learning process, eventually
anticipating the coming heat stimulus.
The
researchers are now investigating details of how attention is physiologically
encoded in the brain. Grover believes the lessons learned from this model
system are likely to directly inform our understanding of human cognition
strategies and neural disorders that disrupt them, but also contribute to new
engineering approaches that lead to performance breakthroughs in artificial
intelligence designs.
The
coauthors of the study include Dhruv Grover, Jen-Yung Chen, Jiayun Xie, Jinfang
Li, Jean-Pierre Changeux and Ralph Greenspan (all affiliated with the UC San
Diego Kavli Institute for Brain and Mind, and J.-P. Changeux also a member of
the Collège de France).