Just do it, research suggests
American Psychological Association
People
consistently underestimate others' desire for constructive feedback and
therefore don't provide it, even when it could improve another person's
performance on a task, according to research published by the American
Psychological Association.This is how NOT to give constructive feedback
"People often have opportunities to provide others with constructive feedback that could be immediately helpful, whether that's letting someone know of a typo in their presentation before a client presentation, or telling a job candidate about a stained shirt before an interview," said lead author Nicole Abi-Esber, a doctoral candidate at Harvard Business School.
"Overall, our research
found that people consistently underestimate others' desire for feedback, which
can have harmful results for would-be feedback recipients."
Constructive feedback is instrumental for aiding learning and performance, and research has shown that people commonly report wanting this type of feedback, according to the researchers. However, despite wanting constructive feedback themselves, people often avoid giving it to others.
In a pilot study conducted by the researchers, only 2.6% of participants informed a tester of a visible smudge on his or her face (e.g., chocolate, lipstick or red marker) during a survey.
The research was
published in APA's Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology.
Previous
research suggests that people avoid giving feedback for fear of negative
outcomes, such as the other person's becoming embarrassed or upset. Abi-Esber
and her colleagues theorized there might be another reason people withhold
feedback: They simply do not fully recognize the potential of their input to
improve others' outcomes, leading them to underestimate others' desire for such
feedback.
To test their theory, the researchers conducted a series of five experiments involving 1,984 participants to measure how much people underestimate others' desire for constructive feedback. In one, participants were presented with 10 hypothetical awkward social situations at work, where they could either give or receive constructive feedback.
In another experiment, participants were asked to recall
a situation where they could either have given or received constructive
feedback. In the final experiment, participants were paired, with one
practicing a speech for a competition and the other assigned to listen and provide
feedback.
Across all five
experiments, people in a position to give feedback consistently underestimated
potential receivers' desire for it. The more consequential the feedback (e.g.,
telling someone they need to improve their presentation skills), the more
likely participants were to underestimate the other's need for feedback and the
less likely they were to offer it. The gap was smaller in more everyday, less
consequential scenarios, such as when the other person had food on their face
or a rip in their pants.
The researchers
were surprised to find that the simple intervention of perspective taking could
increase the likelihood someone would recognize the need for and provide
feedback. Simply asking people to quickly reflect, "If you were this
person, would you want feedback?" helped participants recognize the value
of feedback to the other person and helped close the giver-receiver gap.
"Even if
you feel hesitant to give feedback, we recommend that you give it," said
Abi-Esber. "Take a second and imagine you're in the other person's shoes
and ask yourself if you would want feedback if you were them. Most likely you
would, and this realization can help empower you to give them feedback."
"Feedback
is key to personal growth and improvement, and it can fix problems that are
otherwise costly to the recipient," said co-author Francesca Gino, PhD,
also of Harvard Business School. "The next time you hear someone
mispronounce a word, see a stain on their shirt or notice a typo on their
slide, we urge you to point it out to them -- they probably want feedback more
than you think."