Why people don't view the world the same way others do
University of California - Los Angeles
Why are we so sure that the way we see people, situations and politics is accurate, and the way other people see them is foolishly wrong?
The
answer, according to new research by UCLA psychology professor Matthew
Lieberman, lies in a region of the brain he calls the "gestalt
cortex," which helps people make sense of information that is ambiguous or
incomplete -- and dismiss alternative interpretations.
The
research, based on an analysis of more than 400 previous studies, is published
in the journal Psychological Review.
People often mistake their own understanding of people and events as objective truth, rather than as merely their own interpretation. That phenomenon, called "naive realism," leads people to believe that they should have the final word on the world around them.
"We
tend to have irrational confidence in our own experiences of the world, and to
see others as misinformed, lazy, unreasonable or biased when they fail to see
the world the way we do," Lieberman said. "The evidence from neural
data is clear that the gestalt cortex is central to how we construct our
version of reality."
Naive
realism may be the single most underappreciated source of conflict and distrust
across individuals and groups, he said.
"When
others see the world differently than we do, it can serve as an existential
threat to our own contact with reality and often leads to anger and suspicion
about the others," Lieberman said. "If we know how a person is seeing
the world, their subsequent reactions are much more predictable."
While
the question of how people make sense of the world has been an enduring topic
in social psychology, the underlying brain mechanisms have never been fully
explained, Lieberman said.
Mental
acts that are coherent, effortless and based on our experiences tend to occur
in the gestalt cortex. For example, a person might see someone else smiling and
without giving it any apparent thought, perceive that the other person is
happy. Because those inferences are immediate and effortless, they typically
feel more like "seeing reality" -- even though happiness is an
internal psychological state -- than they do like "thinking,"
Lieberman said.
"We
believe we have merely witnessed things as they are, which makes it more
difficult to appreciate, or even consider, other perspectives," he said.
"The mind accentuates its best answer and discards the rival solutions.
The mind may initially process the world like a democracy where every
alternative interpretation gets a vote, but it quickly ends up like an authoritarian
regime where one interpretation rules with an iron fist and dissent is crushed.
In selecting one interpretation, the gestalt cortex literally inhibits
others."
Previous
research by Lieberman has shown that when people disagree face to face -- for
example on a political issue -- activity in their gestalt cortices is less
similar than it is for people who agree with one another. (That conclusion was
supported by a 2018 study in the journal Nature Communications. UCLA
psychologist Carolyn Parkinson and others found that similar neural patterns in
the gestalt cortex were strong predictors of who was friends with whom.)
Gestalt
was a German school of perceptual psychology whose motto was, "The whole
is greater than the sum of the parts." The approach focused on how the
human mind integrates elements of the world into meaningful groupings.
The
gestalt cortex is located behind the ear, and it is situated between the parts
of the brain responsible for processing vision, sound and touch; those parts
are connected by a structure called the temporoparietal junction, which is part
of the gestalt cortex. In the new study, Lieberman proposes that the
temporoparietal junction is central to conscious experience and that it helps
organize and integrate psychological features of situations that people see so
they can make sense of them effortlessly.
The
gestalt cortex isn't the only area of the brain that enables people to quickly
process and interpret what they see, he said, but it is an especially important
one.
Using
neurosurgical recordings to understand the "social brain"
In
a separate study, published in April in the journal Nature
Communications, Lieberman and colleagues addressed how, given our complex
social worlds, we are able to socialize with relative ease.
Using
the first mass-scale neurosurgical recordings of the "social brain,"
Lieberman, UCLA psychology graduate student Kevin Tan and colleagues at
Stanford University showed that humans have a specialized neural pathway for
social thinking.
Lieberman,
author of the bestselling book "Social: Why Our Brains Are Wired to
Connect," said humans are social by nature and have an exceptional
capacity for assessing the mental states of others. That ability requires the
brain to process a large number of inferences from a vast array of
idiosyncratic cues. So why does that process often feel so effortless compared
to simple tasks like basic arithmetic?
Clear
answers have been elusive for those who study social neuroscience. One culprit
could be scientists' reliance on functional magnetic resonance imaging, which
is effective at scanning where brain activity occurs, but less effective at
capturing the timing of that activity.
Researchers
employed a technique called electrocorticography to record brain activity at
millisecond and millimeter scales using thousands of neurosurgical electrodes.
They found that a neurocognitive pathway that extends from the back to the
front of the brain is especially active in areas closer to the front when
people think about the mental states of others.
Their findings suggest that the temporoparietal junction may create a fast, effortless understanding of other people's mental states, and that another region, the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, may be more involved in thinking things through more slowly and carefully.