Study offers neurological explanation for how brains bias partisans against new information
Brown University
What causes two people from opposing political parties to have strongly divergent interpretations of the same word, image or event?
Take the word “freedom,” for example, or a picture of the American flag, or even the 2020 U.S. presidential election.
A person who identifies politically as liberal
vs. one who identifies as conservative will likely have opposing
interpretations when processing this information — and a new study helps to
explain why.
While
previous theories posited that political polarization results from selective
consumption (and over-consumption) of news and social media, a team led by
researchers at Brown University hypothesized that polarization may start even
earlier.
Their new
study, published in Science Advances, shows that individuals who
share an ideology have more similar neural fingerprints of political words,
experience greater neural synchrony when engaging with political content, and
their brains sequentially segment new information into the same units of
meaning. In this way, the researchers said, they show how polarization arises
at the very point when the brain receives and processes new information.
“This research helps shed light on what happens in the brain that gives rise to political polarization,” said senior study author Oriel FeldmanHall, an associate professor of cognitive, linguistic and psychological sciences who is affiliated with the Carney Institute of Brain Science at Brown University. Daantje de Bruin, a graduate student in FeldmanHall's lab, led the research and conducted the data analysis.
Previous
research from FeldmanHall’s lab showed that when watching a
potentially polarizing video about hot-button issues like abortion, policing or
immigration, the brain activity of people who identified as Democrat or
Republican was similar to the brain activity of people in their respective
parties.
That
neurosynchrony, FeldmanHall explained, is considered evidence that the brains
are processing the information in a similar way. For this new study, the
researchers wanted to get an even more detailed picture of why and how the
brains of people in the same political party are able to sync up.
To
do that, the team used a range of methods that they say have never before been
used in conjunction with each other. They conducted a series of experiments
with a group of 44 participants, equally split among liberals and
conservatives, who agreed to perform various cognitive tasks while undergoing
functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), which measures the small changes
in blood flow that occur with brain activity.
This
research helps shed light on what happens in the brain that gives rise to
political polarization.
Participants
first completed a word reading task in which they were presented with single
words (e.g., “immigration,” “abortion”) and asked to determine whether the word
was political or non-political (indicated via a button press). Then the
participants watched a series of videos, including a neutrally worded news clip
on abortion and a heated 2016 vice presidential campaign debate on police
brutality and immigration. During the experiments, the participants’ brain
activity was measured using fMRI.
One
of the methods the researchers used is called representation similarity
analysis. When a person sees a simple, static image, like a word, the brain
will represent that word with certain activity patterns.
“You
can think of it as the brain representing the word by firing neurons in a
certain way,” FeldmanHall said. “It’s almost like a fingerprint — a neural
fingerprint that encodes the concept of that word within the brain.”
She
added that since neural activity patterns store information about the world,
how the brain represents this information is considered a metric for how that
information is interpreted and used to steer behavior and attitudes.
In
the study, the participants were exposed to words that are often politicized,
like “abortion,” “immigration” and “gangs,” as well as more ambiguous words,
like “freedom”.
The
researchers found by analyzing the fMRI data that the neural fingerprint
created by a liberal brain is more similar to other liberal brains than the
neural fingerprint created by a conservative brain, and vice versa. This is
important, FeldmanHall said, because it shows how the brains of partisans are
processing information in a polarized way, even when it’s devoid of any
political context.
Putting
the polarized pieces together to create an ideological story
The
researchers also used a newer methodology called neural segmentation to explore
how the brains of people who identify with a particular party bias the
interpretation of incoming information. Brains are constantly receiving visual
and auditory input, FeldmanHall said, and the way the brain makes sense of that
continuous barrage of information is to separate it into discrete chunks, or
segments.
“It's
like dividing a book of solid text into sentences, paragraphs and chapters,”
she said.
The
researchers found that the brains of Democrats separate incoming information in
the same way, which then gives similar, partisan meanings to those pieces of
information — but that the brains of Republicans segment the same information
in a different way.
The
researchers noted that individuals who shared an ideology had more similar
neural representations of political words and experienced greater neural
synchrony while watching the political videos, and segmented real-world
information into the same meaningful units.
“The
reason two liberal brains are synchronizing when watching a complicated video
is due in part to the fact that each brain has neural fingerprints for
political concepts or words that are very aligned,” FeldmanHall explained.
This
explains why two opposing partisans can watch the same news segment and both
believe that it was biased against their side — for each partisan, the words,
images, sounds and concepts were represented in their brain in a different way
(but similar to other partisans who share their ideology). The stream of
information was also segmented out in a different format, telling a different
ideological story.
Taken
together, the researchers concluded, the findings show that political ideology
is shaped by semantic representations of political concepts processed in an
environment free of any polarizing agenda, and that these representations bias
how real-world political information is construed into a polarized perspective.
“In
this way, our study provided a mechanistic account for why political
polarization arises,” FeldmanHall said.
The
researchers are now focusing on how this explanation of polarization can be
used to combat polarization.
“The
problem of political polarization can’t be addressed on a superficial level,”
FeldmanHall said. “Our work showed that these polarized beliefs are very
entrenched, and go all the way down to the way people experience a political
word. Understanding this will influence how researchers think about potential
interventions.”
Additional
contributors to this research included Pedro L. Rodríguezfrom the Center for
Data Science at New York University and Jeroen M. van Baar from the Netherlands
Institute of Mental Health and Addiction.