Time is on your side
University of Nevada, Las Vegas
Ever hear the old adage that time flies when you're
having fun? A new study by a team of UNLV researchers suggests that there's a
lot of truth to the trope.Illustration by UNLV graduate student and study co-author Talha K. Soluoku
Many people think of their brains as being intrinsically
synced to the human-made clocks on their electronic devices, counting time in
very specific, minute-by-minute increments. But the study, published this month
in the latest issue of the peer-reviewed Cell Press journal Current
Biology, showed that our brains don't work that way.
By analyzing changes in brain activity patterns, the
research team found that we perceive the passage of time based on the number of
experiences we have -- not some kind of internal clock. What's more, increasing
speed or output during an activity appears to affect how our brains perceive
time.
"We tell time in our own experience by things we do, things that happen to us," said James Hyman, a UNLV associate professor of psychology and the study's senior author. "When we're still and we're bored, time goes very slowly because we're not doing anything or nothing is happening. On the contrary, when a lot of events happen, each one of those activities is advancing our brains forward. And if this is how our brains objectively tell time, then the more that we do and the more that happens to us, the faster time goes."
Methodology and Findings
The findings are based on analysis of activity in the
anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), a portion of the brain important for
monitoring activity and tracking experiences. To do this, rodents were tasked
with using their noses to respond to a prompt 200 times.
Scientists already knew that brain patterns are similar,
but slightly different, each time you do a repetitive motion, so they set out
to answer: Is it possible to detect whether these slight differences in brain
pattern changes correspond with doing the first versus 200th motion in series?
And does the amount of time it takes to complete a series of motions impact
brain wave activity?
By comparing pattern changes throughout the course of the task, researchers observed that there are indeed detectable changes in brain activity that occur as one moves from the beginning to middle to end of carrying out a task. And regardless of how slowly or quickly the animals moved, the brain patterns followed the same path.
The patterns were consistent when
researchers applied a machine learning-based mathematical model to predict the
flow of brain activity, bolstering evidence that it's experiences -- not time,
or a prescribed number of minutes, as you would measure it on a clock -- that
produce changes in our neurons' activity patterns.
Hyman drove home the crux of the findings by sharing an
anecdote of two factory workers tasked with making 100 widgets during their
shift, with one worker completing the task in 30 minutes and the other in 90
minutes.
"The length of time it took to complete the task
didn't impact the brain patterns. The brain is not a clock; it acts like a
counter," Hyman explained. "Our brains register a vibe, a feeling
about time. ...And what that means for our workers making widgets is that you
can tell the difference between making widget No. 85 and widget No. 60, but not
necessarily between No. 85 and No. 88."
But exactly "how" does the brain
count? Researchers discovered that as the brain progresses through a task
involving a series of motions, various small groups of firing cells begin to
collaborate -- essentially passing off the task to a different group of neurons
every few repetitions, similar to runners passing the baton in a relay race.
"So, the cells are working together and over time
randomly align to get the job done: one cell will take a few tasks and then
another takes a few tasks," Hyman said. "The cells are tracking
motions and, thus, chunks of activities and time over the course of the
task."
And the study's findings about our brains' perception of
time applies to activities-based actions other than physical motions too.
"This is the part of the brain we use for tracking
something like a conversation through dinner," Hyman said. "Think of
the flow of conversation and you can recall things earlier and later in the
dinner. But to pick apart one sentence from the next in your memory, it's
impossible. But you know you talked about one topic at the start, another topic
during dessert, and another at the end."
By observing the rodents who worked quickly, scientists
also concluded that keeping up a good pace helps influence time perception:
"The more we do, the faster time moves. They say that time flies when
you're having fun. As opposed to having fun, maybe it should be 'time flies
when you're doing a lot'."
Takeaways
While there's already a wealth of information on brain
processes over very short time scales of less than a second, Hyman said that
the UNLV study is groundbreaking in its examination of brain patterns and
perception of time over a span of just a few minutes to hours -- "which is
how we live much of our life: one hour at a time. "
"This is among the first studies looking at
behavioral time scales in this particular part of the brain called the ACC,
which we know is so important for our behavior and our emotions," Hyman
said.
The ACC is implicated in most psychiatric and
neurodegenerative disorders, and is a concentration area for mood disorders,
PTSD, addiction, and anxiety. ACC function is also central to various dementias
including Alzheimer's disease, which is characterized by distortions in time.
The ACC has long been linked to helping humans with sequencing events or tasks
such as following recipes, and the research team speculates that their findings
about time perception might fall within this realm.
While the findings are a breakthrough, more research is
needed. Still, Hyman said, the preliminary findings posit some potentially
helpful tidbits about time perception and its likely connection to memory
processes for everyday citizens' daily lives. For example, researchers
speculate that it could lend insights for navigating things like school
assignments or even breakups.
"If we want to remember something, we may want to
slow down by studying in short bouts and take time before engaging in the next
activity. Give yourself quiet times to not move," Hyman said.
"Conversely, if you want to move on from something quickly, get involved
in an activity right away."
Hyman said there's also a huge relationship between the
ACC, emotion, and cognition. Thinking of the brain as a physical entity that
one can take ownership over might help us control our subjective experiences.
"When things move faster, we tend to think it's more fun -- or sometimes overwhelming. But we don't need to think of it as being a purely psychological experience, as fun or overwhelming; rather, if you view it as a physical process, it can be helpful," he said. "If it's overwhelming, slow down or if you're bored, add activities. People already do this, but it's empowering to know it's a way to work your own mental health, since our brains are working like this already."